Craniofacial Region is the Dominant Site in Response to Audio-Visual Contagious Itch in Healthy Humans: An Experimental Study
Fatima Palani1, Khatema Waziri1,
Parisa Gazerani2*
1School of Medicine and
Health, Faculty of Medicine, Aalborg University, Aalborg, Denmark
2Biomedicine, Department of Health Science and Technology,
Faculty of Medicine, Aalborg University, Aalborg, Denmark
*Corresponding author: Parisa Gazerani,
Biomedicine, Department of Health Science and Technology, Aalborg University,
Fredrik Bajers Vej 3B, room 2.205, DK-9220 Aalborg E, Denmark. Tel:
+4599402412; Email: gazerani@hst.aau.dk
Received Date: 24 August, 2018; Accepted Date: 04
September, 2018; Published Date: 12
September, 2018
Citation: Palani F, Waziri K, Gazerani P (2018) Craniofacial Region is the Dominant Site in Response to Audio-Visual Contagious
Itch in Healthy Humans: An Experimental Study. Clin Exp Dermatol Ther: CEDT-153. DOI: 10.29011/2575-8268/100053
1. Abstract
Introduction: Contagious
Itch (CI) is referred to the experience of itch sensation when observing
itching or scratching behavior in others or listening to the topic. This
phenomenon is observed in humans and animals (monkeys and mice).
Cortico-striatal neuronal circuit has been proposed to contribute to CI.
Involvement of mirror neurons has also been suggested. We established an
experimental audio-visual CI model to explore the impact of body region
(craniofacial, arm, back, chest), sex, and sound.
Discussion: Audio-visual itch model was efficient in provoking CI in healthy young adults. Females rated itch intensity higher than males regardless of the body region. Craniofacial region was the most dominant site regardless of sex. Males and females responded differently to sound, where males were more sensitive to audio-visual stimuli.
2. Introduction
Contagious Itch (CI) is a concept that is referred to the
experience of itch sensation when observing itching or scratching behavior in
others or even by listening to a discussion related to the topic [1-3].
This phenomenon is not only seen in humans, but also in animals [4]. CI is
also relatable to another universal concept, so-called contagious
yawning [5]. However, CI has only been subjected to investigation since
2000. Niemeier et al. [1] were the first to reveal that itch can be
induced by visual cues. In their study, a group of audiences viewed two
presentations of images; the first one was an itch-evoking lecture (images of
insects, scratch marks and allergic reactions), whereas the second presentation
concentrated on relaxation (pictures of baby skin and children). This study
demonstrated a significant increase in scratching when observing the “itch
lecture” in comparison to the “relaxation lecture” [1]. Several years later,
another study [6] approached an almost identical procedure, aiming to
investigate if visual stimuli without auditory cues would evoke itch. They
asked groups of students to watch video clips to provoke coldness, pain, or
itch. The itch-evoking video depicted images of head lice moving and people
scratching their heads. This itch-evoking video led to more scratching and
higher levels of itch compared to the videos depicting coldness and
pain [6]. Two years later, another study [3] applied visual
stimulus to induce itch. This group presented a video of a person scratching
himself to a control group and a group of Atopic Dermatitis (AD) patients. Both
groups showed an increased scratching behavior by watching the experimental
video compared to the control video with the neutral content [3]. However,
the AD group rated a significantly higher itch intensity compared to the
control group. This indicates that AD patients are more susceptible to
itch-inducing audio-visual stimuli in contrast to healthy controls [7].
Another study in 2013 [8] investigated if visual cue could provoke
itching and scratching response in healthy females using different images. The
participants were presented two different Power Points, an itch related (e.g.
ants, fleas or skin conditions) and a neutral one (e.g. butterflies or healthy
skin). The presentation slides were moreover divided into types such as ‘skin
contact’ (e.g. ants crawling on the hand or a butterfly on a finger), ‘skin
response’ (e.g. scratching an insect bite or washing the hands) or ‘context
only’ (e.g. viewing midges or birds flying). The study showed an increased itch
intensity when viewing the itch related presentation compared to the neural
presentation [8].
Besides human studies, animal studies of CI have also been
conducted. For instance, a group of researchers [4] demonstrated CI
in monkeys who observed itching and scratching behavior of other monkeys
represented in videos. A recent study in non-mammals (mice) also presented
similar results, where mice presented scratching behavior after noticing
scratching animals in an adjacent cage [9-11]. This finding indicates that
not only humans are prone to CI, but also non-human primates and non-mammals’
mice can present CI behavior [9-11].
The neural mechanism
of CI has been examined by two prior fMRI studies [12,13] proving activity in
several brain regions, such as Insular Cortex (IC), Supplementary
Motor Area (SMA), Premotor Cortex (PM), and Prefrontal
Cortex (PFC) during observation of other individuals sensing itch. Investigating
the role of empathy in pain, similar brain regions have been found
activated [14,15]. Anterior Insular Cortex (aIC) is associated with
empathy for itch. Another group [13] set up an experiment, analysing
brain activity of subjects imaging the itch sensation while watching images of
itching skin and at the same time images of painful skin. They wanted to
further test the empathy and role of aIC. Identical activation processes were
observed; however, there was a difference in functional connectivity between
itch and pain in brain images. Additionally, there was a significant increase
in functional connectivity between the aIC and basal ganglia during the itch
sensation. The basal ganglia is composed of an anatomical circuit including
areas such as the SMA, PM, and MI. This
circuit functions in motor control. The aIC is anatomically related to the
basal ganglia [16] and if any lesions
appear in the aIC, motivation and craving in general are inhibited [17].
Hence, a potential process behind the scratching reaction that occurs when observing
others could be that activation of aIC that motivates directly or indirectly
motor activity in the cortico-striatal circuit via the basal ganglia leading to
scratching response. Another possible mechanism underlying CI could potentially
be functional coupling between aIC and global pallidus
(GP) [13]. GP has the function of encouraging acts and
“goal-directed behavior” [18,19]. This finding could propose that
dissimilarity in the functional coupling may explain the reason behind
increased motor response, while observing itch in others. A former fMRI study
in CI has demonstrated that during itch stimuli (viewing others itching and
scratching), SI was significantly activated [12] whereas this region
in pain studies is found to associated with empathy for pain. Two additional
brain imaging studies focused on somatic hallucination and found a significant
activation of the medial parietal cortex along with the posterior cingulate
cortex and precuneus [20,21]. Precuneus plays an important role in
memory [22]; hence, the memory of experiencing itch may potentially
contribute to the underlying mechanism of itch transmission while viewing
others’ itch experience or scratching. Moreover, there is a non-mammal
study [10] that has looked deeper into the underlying mechanisms of
CI. This study demonstrated, by molecular mapping, a greater neural activation
in the Suprachiasmatic Nucleus (SCN) of the
observer mice experiencing scratching sensation when observing another mice
scratching. The study concluded that depletion of gastrin-releasing
peptide (GRP) receptor (GRPR) or GRPR neurons in SCN inhibits
contagious scratching behavior whereas excitation of GRP/GRPR neurons could
improve scratching behavior, proposing that GRP-GRPR signalling is an important
pathway in CI [10].
The underlying
mechanism of CI is not completely understood; hence, different hypotheses have
been made; one of which is the activation of Mirror Neurons (MNs). MNs are a
specific group of neurons that are activated when performing a motor act and
imitating others executing a similar motor act. MNs have become more and more
popular due to its contribution to elucidation of social behavior, imitation,
language processing and other parameters like empathy, emotion recognition and
intention-reading, etc. No association has yet been established between CI in
human and MNs; however, one study [4] has reported that CI is a
common phenomenon in primates. This could indicate that MNs may also play a
role in CI in humans. By reviewing the experiments where abstract stimuli have
been approached to induce itch, it can be speculated that itch sensation that
is occurred due to itch depicting images, may be involved in activating the
sensory-emotional elements of the MNs. The elements of MNs include Anterior Cingulate Cortex (ACC) and the Anterior Insula, and
these brain regions are commonly found in studies related to pain
empathy [23]. Such findings emphasize that pain and itch are not only
physiologically closely related but that psychology of pain and itch also
overlaps in some aspects [24].
CI is still a less
studied area and there are still number of open questions in the field that
require further investigation. Hence, we established an experimental CI model
to explore the impact of sex and body region on CI. We compared responses of
craniofacial region to other body regions such as arm, back, and chest in
females and males. We also investigated if any significant difference exists in
itch intensity when observers are exposed to visual itch stimuli with and without
scratching sound. We hypothesized that participants would be more susceptible
to CI within craniofacial region compared with arm, chest, and back. We also
proposed that visual cues with sound would have a higher impact reflected on
higher itch intensity rated by the participants. As females tend to show more
empathy [25] than men, we also hypothesized that a sex-related
response would be detectable with females being more prone to visual itch
stimuli.
3. Methods
3.1. Participants
A group of 20 healthy
participants (10 females and 10 males), 18-34 years (26.15±4.66 years
(Mean±SD)) were recruited. Exclusion criteria included 1) any previous or
skin-related conditions 2) allergic disorders 3) any ongoing itch 4) rashes or
lesions on the particular body regions and 5) any pain or discomfort at the day
of the experiment. All participants signed a written informed consent before
participating in the experiment in accordance with the 2013 version of the
Helsinki Declaration [26]. The study protocol was communicated with the
regional ethics committee of the Northern region in Denmark for obtaining
ethical approval, but it was confirmed that there was no necessity for an
approval on conduction of this study due to the nature of this study.
A randomized controlled
cross-over study was designed. The study consisted of three steps. First step
included a questionnaire, which had to be filled out before the experiment.
Second step was presentation of the two Experimental Videos (EV), both with and
without sound presented in a randomized order based on sex and sound to avoid a
potential bias. Fifteen minutes of washout period between each video
presentation was established to avoid a carry-over effect. Third step consisted
of the two Control Videos (CV) presentation composed of a neutral content,
where the same demonstrators were recorded but in a relaxed and idle condition.
The participants were seated in a quiet room without any distractions, and the
videos were presented on a computer and when the videos with sound were
presented, headphones were provided. Participants were instructed to rate itch
intensity on a visual analogue scale (VAS 0-10) after each video and to draw
the itching areas on the body chart (Figure 1).
3.2. Questionnaire
The questionnaire was composed
of questions based on demographic variables, such as sex, age and personality
traits including extroversion, neuroticism, openness to experiences,
agreeableness and consciousness, which was based on the so-called Five Factor
Model (FFM) [27]. Additional questions were asked about whether the
participants received any medications, and how they would describe their mood
at the time of experiment.
3.3. Videos
Four videos were
recorded by the investigators capturing demonstrators either scratching themselves
or sitting relaxed. Each video was of 5 minutes length. Two of the videos
displayed a female demonstrator while the other two videos displayed a male
demonstrator. The demonstrators were instructed to itch and scratch specific
areas, arm, chest, back and craniofacial region. Two videos (female-male) were
dedicated to controls and two (female-male) to the scratching behaviour. The EV
depicted either a female or male demonstrator itching and scratching, whereas
the CV depicted the same demonstrators, but in a relaxed and idle condition.
3.4. Body
Charts
Body charts were used
to mark on 4 target sites of arm, chest, back and craniofacial region for
itching sensation following exposure to the videos.
3.5. Rating
of Itch
A VAS scale anchored
with 0 and 10 (0=no itch, 10=extreme itch) was provided in order to rate the
intensity of itch.
3.6. Statistical
Analysis
Data were analysed by Sigma Plot 14.0. A normality test was
performed using the Shapiro-Wilk test. P-values ≤ 0.05 were
considered as significant. A three-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was applied
to compare itch intensity on VAS based on 3 factors: sex (with two levels; male
and female), location (with four levels; craniofacial region, chest, arm and
back) and sound (with two levels; with and without sound). If ANOVA results
showed a significant difference, post-hoc test, Holm-Sidak, was applied to identify where the
exact difference was located. Data are presented as means and standard
deviation, standard error of the mean, or percentages in text and figures,
unless otherwise stated.
4. Results
All enrolled participants completed the experiments. Out of 20 participants, 4 reported some itchiness while watching the CV with and without sound. These 4 participants did not show a variation in rating response.
Data analysis revealed that CI could be induced in healthy participants. A main effect of sex was found (ANOVA, (F(1,288)=7.691, P< 0.006) where females were more sensitive than males (Figure 2). There was also a significant main effect of location (ANOVA, F(3,288)=8.706, p < 0.001), indicating that body regions show different susceptibility to perception of CI (Figure 2). Holm-Sidak post hoc yielded a statistically significant interaction between females and the craniofacial region (P<0.001), suggesting that among the four regions, craniofacial region was most sensitive to CI in females (Figure 2). However, this was not the case for males. Within the craniofacial region, a statistically significant difference (P=0.04) was also observed between male and female observers, indicating that itch intensity was more pronounced in the craniofacial region in females (Figure 2). However, no significant main effect of sound was yielded, (ANOVA, F(3,288)=0.779, p < 0.507), indicating that sound overall did not influence the itch sensation under conditions of this experiment
Looking into
interactions of the sound results, it was evident that for females, videos of a
female demonstrator without sound, was significantly different from males
(p=0.04), which indicates that female observers were more sensitive to CI
without sound compared to male observers.
Moreover, there was a
statistically significant difference within the craniofacial region vs
chest region (P=<0.001) and craniofacial vs arm (P=0.004) when EV depicting
male demonstrator was presented with sound. This indicates that the
craniofacial region was more prone to CI than chest and arm, but still with an
influence of sex.
5. Discussion
The purpose of the
present study was to further investigate the phenomenon of CI and that whether
audio-visual stimuli had any additional impact on healthy participants in
particular in relation to body regions. Additional factors, such as sex and
sound were also examined. Overall, the results from this exploratory study
confirm that audio-visual stimuli depicting itch-related scenarios can evoke
itch in healthy adult individuals, the phenomenon is sex-related, and the
craniofacial region is more sensitive. Below, the findings are discussed in
more details.
5.1. Sex
The current study showed
that females exhibit more sensitivity to itch by rating higher scores on VAS
compared to males when exposed to the audio-visual stimulation. Female also
expressed higher itch intensity in the craniofacial region compared to males.
Based on a former study [25],
it has been proposed that females express higher empathy, which makes them
better at relating and empathizing with the internal emotional conditions of
others; hence, this might be an explanation as why females are more responsive
to visual itch stimulation. One possible mechanism underlying this empathic
process is the MNs [28],
which is arguably an inherent mechanism that captures actions of the
surroundings. This idea [3] states that
contribution of MNs might be a built-in mechanism, since CI occurs following to
visual cue, in healthy individuals and in AD patients, though in lower
magnitude, comparatively. This indicates that activation of MNs may be
amplified in AD patients during visual itch stimuli [3]. According
to Ferrari et al. [29] MNs
have immense control over facial motor acts (biting, sucking); hence, MNs may
also contribute in CI [18,30,31]. Alternative
possibilities behind the difference in itch response between the sexes could be
due to difference in thresholds scratching. This means that some people feel
itchy, but their motor action of scratching comes later than others.
5.2. Location
The present study also
showed that the craniofacial region was mostly affected by the audio-visual
stimuli. Based on the previous studies, it has been reported that body parts
that response to itch visual cue vary between chronic itch patients and healthy
controls. A study from 2011 [3] reported that AD
patients scratched body regions distal from the body parts that were scratched
in the videos; while the healthy control scratched body parts, proximal to the
body regions that were scratched in the presented video. This evidence is
supported by another study [32], where
healthy participants in their study showed an increased itch perception at
proximal body site (head), despite viewing scratching of the chest and arm.
Interestingly, macaque monkeys [4] exhibit
almost identical scratching behavior as humans, when exposed to video
presentations of other monkey executing scratching behavior. Taken together,
these findings indicate that CI is not linked to one specific body location,
but some locations might be more sensitive than others. Further investigation
is required to substantiate our findings of CI in the craniofacial region.
5.3. Sound
Effect
This study investigated
whether itch perception would be affected by audio stimuli and the results
showed that the audio parameter overall does not have any impact on itch perception
in participants. Therefore, visual stimuli can be considered the main drive for
CI. However, the itch sensation was evidently more increased in the
craniofacial region comparatively to the chest and arm, while observing the EV
with the male demonstrator presented with sound. Therefore, effect of sound
might be sex-dependent and also region-dependent. This needs further
investigation. Similar results have been reported in a previous study [33],
where the authors concluded that audio increased itch susceptibility both in
the psoriasis and healthy group without peripheral stimulation (e.g. ants
crawling or insects bites). A potential explanation on why sound could have an
additional effect to the visual cue is that a potential mechanism where the motor
execution of scratching and connected somatosensory sensations of certain body
parts are simulated/imitated in the observers’ brain, which triggers the
auditory MNs [33]. The
MNs contains the so-called area F5, which is composed of audio-visual MNs. These
neurons are fired not only when observing a movement execution but also when
only the sound of the same movement is captured [34]. This
potential function of MNs suggests an audio-visual motor association between
visualization, motor act, and sound [35]. This
might also explain the results of the present study, indicating that this area
might have been activated during the EV with the sound of scratching and
visualizing of itch at the same location.
Higher cortical regions
might also be involved in the overall response. For instance, unpleasantness of
itch sensation, which is an affective aspect of itch, may share similar region
in the brain, insula, for reaction. Overall response in CI is not a simple
result of motor act and body region but a higher cortical response. Therefore,
currently we could only present that female and males responded differently to
sound, but the mechanism underlying such effect is not clear to explain. Since
females- by nature - are more empathetic in general they might have related
more to the demonstrators in the EV even without sound compared to males [25,36].
5.4. Study
Limitations and Future Perspectives
For the analysis, only
the sex of the observers was taken into consideration, and the sex of the
demonstrator was considered only in combination with the videos, either with or
without sound. Since it has been shown that in pain, both the sex of subjects, and
investigators can influence the outcome of pain intensity [37],
future studies with larger population could also test this hypothesis for CI.
This would be in particular interesting as in clinic sex related differences in
itch have been shown [38] and
human experimental models of itch have also demonstrated sex-related
differences in response to evoked itch [39].
It is important to
emphasize that besides sex, locations, and sound, personality trait is another
potential factor that can conceptualize one’s susceptibility to audio-visual
itch stimuli. Former studies [12,40,41] have
proposed that among healthy young adults, neuroticism is the most common
personality characteristic and that can affect responsiveness to sensory
stimuli. Additional factors that have been linked with increased itch
intensities, involve negative emotions such as anxiety. One study [6] has
found a correlation between itch and anxiety. In this study, the students who
felt itchiness also expressed anxiety, which may indicate that people with a
certain mood can be affected by itch differently. Another example is the study
by van Laarhoven et al. [42], who
reported that females in positive emotional condition experienced reduced itch
intensity compared to those with negative emotions. It is noteworthy to mention
that there are several other studies that have been able to reveal strong
correlations between personality traits and itch perception in healthy and
patients with skin-related conditions [7]. Even
though these results propose a correlation between negative emotions and
personality traits and an increase in itch intensities, personality trait
should only be treated as a factor that can exacerbate the experience of itch
and not as a potential cause of developing itchy skin-disorder [7]. Due
to the relatively small sample size, we could not manage to establish the
impact of personality trait on CI. The personality traits of the female
participants were as follows 30% expressed extroversion, 20% consciousness, 20%
agreeableness, 20% openness and 10% neuroticism. Male participants were
presented as 70% extroversion, 20% consciousness and 10% agreeableness.
The expletory nature
of this study in addition to a relatively small sample size, did not allow us
to establish the impact of age on CI. Age might be an influencing factor in
responsiveness to CI. Additionally, it is not known whether there is any
difference in susceptibility to CI depending on race, and hence a heterogeneous
group should be approached in future studies. We did not account for the time
of cycle in females. Impact of hormones on CI could be taken into
considerations with a potential of fluctuation hormonal pattern during
menstrual cycle on CI responsiveness in females.
6. Conclusion
Our study confirmed
that audio-visual itch model was efficient in provoking CI in healthy young
adults. Females rated itch intensity higher than males regardless of the body
region. Craniofacial region was the most dominant site regardless of sex. Males
and females responded differently to sound, where males were more sensitive to
audio-visual stimuli.
7. Acknowledgements
Fatima Palani and Khatema Waziri made
equal contributions to the conduction of the study and preparation of the
manuscript.
We warmly thank all
study participants for their voluntarily dedication of time and contribution to
this research work.
Figure 1: Experimental setup. Each participant has been exposed to two EVs with a male and a female demonstrator, respectively. Both EVs were presented with and without sound with a washout period of 15 minutes in between, and a presentation of control video. EV: Experimental video. Please note that the order of the tests was randomized.
Figure 2: Illustration of the effect of
audio-visual-evoked itch in healthy male and female participants rating itch
intensity on VAS (0-10) and the effect of audio-visual-induced itch on the
specific body regions, including craniofacial region, chest, back and arm. F:
Female, M: Male. Data are presented as Mean±SEM (Standard Error of the
Mean).
- Niemeier V, Kupfer J, Gieler U (2000) Observations during an Itch-Inducing Lecture. Dermatol Psychosom 1: 15-18.
- Hosogi M, Schmelz M, Miyachi Y, Ikoma A (2006) Bradykinin is a potent pruritogen in atopic dermatitis: A switch from pain to itch. Pain 126: 16-23.
- Papoiu AD, Wang H, Coghill RC, Chan YH, Yosipovitch G (2011) Contagious itch in humans: a study of visual 'transmission' of itch in atopic dermatitis and healthy subjects. Br J Dermatol 164: 1299-1303.
- Feneran AN, O'Donnell R, Press A, Yosipovitch G, Cline M, et al. (2013) Monkey See, Monkey Do: Contagious Itch in Nonhuman Primates. Acta Dermato-Venereologica 93: 27-29.
- Platek SM, Mohamed FB, Gallup GG Jr (2005) Contagious yawning and the brain. Cognitive Brain Research 23: 448-452.
- Ogden J, Zoukas S (2009) Generating physical symptoms from visual cues: An experimental study. Psychol Health Med 14: 695-704.
- Schut C, Grossman S, Gieler U, Kupfer J, Yosipovitch G (2015) Contagious itch: what we know and what we would like to know. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9: 57.
- Lloyd DM, Hall E, Hall S, McGlone FP (2013) Can itch-related visual stimuli alone provoke a scratch response in healthy individuals? British Journal of Dermatology 168: 106-111.
- Liljencrantz J, Pitcher MH, Low LA, Bauer L, Bushnell MC (2017) Comment on "Molecular and neural basis of contagious itch behavior in mice". Science 357: (6347).
- Yu YQ, Barry DM, Hao Y, Liu XT, Chen ZF (2017) Molecular and neural basis of contagious itch behavior in mice. Science 355: 1072-1076.
- Barry DM, Yu YQ, Hao Y, Liu XT, Chen ZF (2017) Response to Comment on "Molecular and neural basis of contagious itch behavior in mice". Science 357: (6347).
- Holle H, Warne K, Seth AK, Critchley HD, Ward J (2012) Neural basis of contagious itch and why some people are more prone to it. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109: 19816-19821.
- Mochizuki H, Baumgärtner U, Kamping S, Ruttorf M, Schad LR, et al. (2013) Cortico-subcortical activation patterns for itch and pain imagery. Pain 154: 1989-1998.
- Perry A, Bentin S, Bartal IB, Lamm C, Decety J (2010) "Feeling" the pain of those who are different from us: Modulation of EEG in the mu/alpha range. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 10: 493-504.
- Press C, Richardson D, Bird G (2010) Intact imitation of emotional facial actions in autism spectrum conditions. Neuropsychologia 48: 3291-3297.
- Flynn FG (1999) Anatomy of the insula - functional and clinical correlates. Aphasiology 13: 55-78.
- Naqvi NH, Rudrauf D, Damasio H, Bechara A (2007) Damage to the insula disrupts addiction to cigarette smoking. Science 315: 531-534.
- Miller JM, Vorel SR, Tranguch AJ, Kenny ET, Mazzoni P,et al. (2006) Anhedonia after a selective bilateral lesion of the globus pallidus. American Journal of Psychiatry 163: 786-788.
- Adam R, Leff A, Sinha N, Turner C, Bays P, et al. (2013) Dopamine reverses reward insensitivity in apathy following globus pallidus lesions. Cortex 49: 1292-1303.
- Shergill SS, Cameron LA, Brammer MJ, Williams SC, Murray RM, et al. (2001) Modality specific neural correlates of auditory and somatic hallucinations. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 71: 688-690.
- Bär KJ, Gaser C, Nenadic I, Sauer H (2002) Transient activation of a somatosensory area in painful hallucinations shown by fMRI. Neuroreport 13: 805-808.
- Cavanna AE, Trimble MR (2006) The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 129: 564-583.
- Acharya S, Shukla S (2012) Mirror neurons: Enigma of the metaphysical modular brain. J Nat Sci Biol Med 3: 118-124.
- Schmelz M (2015) Itch and pain differences and commonalities. Handb Exp Pharmacol 227: 285-301.
- Wilson SE, Prescott J, Becket G (2012) Empathy Levels in First- and Third-Year Students in Health and Non-Health Disciplines. American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education 76: 24.
- Muthuswamy V (2014) The new 2013 seventh version of the Declaration of Helsinki--more old wine in a new bottle? Indian J Med Ethics 11: 2-4.
- McCrae RR, Costa PT Jr, Pedroso de Lima M, Simões A, Ostendorf F, et al. (1999) Age differences in personality across the adult life span: Parallels in five cultures. Developmental Psychology 35: 466-477.
- Iacoboni M (2009) Imitation, Empathy, and Mirror Neurons. Annual Review of Psychology 60: 653-670.
- Ferrari PF, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L (2003) Mirror neurons responding to the observation of ingestive and communicative mouth actions in the monkey ventral premotor cortex. European Journal of Neuroscience 17: 1703-1714.
- Haker H, Kawohl W, Herwig U, Rössler W (2013) Mirror neuron activity during contagious yawning-an fMRI study. Brain Imaging and Behavior 7: 28-34.
- Gallup AC, Eldakar OT (2013) The thermoregulatory theory of yawning: what we know from over 5 years of research. Frontiers in Neuroscience 6: 188.
- Ward J, Burckhardt V, Holle H (2013) Contagious scratching: shared feelings but not shared body locations. Front Hum Neurosci 7: 122.
- Swithenbank S, Cowdell F, Holle H (2016) The Role of Auditory Itch Contagion in Psoriasis. Acta Derm Venereol 96: 728-731.
- Kohler E, Keysers C, Umiltà MA, Fogassi L, Gallese V, et al. (2002) Hearing sounds, understanding actions: action representation in mirror neurons. Science 297: 846-848.
- Oberman LM, Hubbard EM, McCleery JP, Altschuler EL, Ramachandran VS, et al. (2005) EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 24: 190-198.
- Ständer S, Weisshaar E, Mettang T, Szepietowski JC, Carstens E, et al. (2007) Clinical classification of itch: a position paper of the International Forum for the Study of Itch. Acta Derm Venereol 87: 291-294.
- Fillingim RB, King CD, Ribeiro-Dasilva MC, Rahim-Williams B, et al. (2009) Sex, gender, and pain: a review of recent clinical and experimental findings. J Pain 10: 447-485.
- Ständer S, Stumpf A, Osada N, Wilp S, Chatzigeorgakidis E , et al. (2013) Gender differences in chronic pruritus: women present different morbidity, more scratch lesions and higher burden. Br J Dermatol 168: 1273-1280.
- Hartmann EM, Handwerker HO, Forster C (2015) Gender differences in itch and pain-related sensations provoked by histamine, cowhage and capsaicin. Acta Derm Venereol 95: 25-30.
- Fredborg B, Clark J, Smith SD (2017)An Examination of Personality Traits Associated with Autonomous Sensory Meridian Response (ASMR). Frontiers in Psychology 8: 247.
- Jagiellowicz J, Xu X, Aron A, Aron E, Cao G, et al. (2011) The trait of sensory processing sensitivity and neural responses to changes in visual scenes. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 6: 38-47.
- van Laarhoven AI, Walker AL, Wilder-Smith OH, Kroeze S, van Riel PL, et al. (2012) Role of induced negative and positive emotions in sensitivity to itch and pain in women. British Journal of Dermatology 167: 262-269.
© by the Authors & Gavin Publishers. This is an Open Access Journal Article Published Under Attribution-Share Alike CC BY-SA: Creative Commons Attribution-Share Alike 4.0 International License. Read More About Open Access Policy.